American University Communication Discussion

Can you complete one of the three questions? Only one question needs to be answered in about 200 words

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

The stage of adolescence is a susceptible time for teenagers. During this age, many adolescents become more cautious of their appearances and identity, which may lead to influence from peers, TV, magazines, and social media. Beye s et al. (2020) conducted a mixed-method (quantitative and qualitative) study to determine whether an increase in an adolescent’s social media use is associated with an increase or decrease in that adolescent’s well-being. Also, the researchers wanted to know if passive or active use of social media use played a factor in adolescents’ well-being based on different social media platforms. Passive use refers to consuming information without direct exchanges, and active use is engaging with others on social media, like commenting or posting pictures.

The researchers focus their study on a within-person association study compared to four recent studies that use the same study design with different indicators. The “literature review” is structured in two cohesive subheadings to provide definitions of the indicators for the current study, such as person-specific effects and social media and affective well-being. Beyens et al. (2020) sought to determine if Person-specific effects mean the “effect of social media differs from adolescent to adolescent.” Also, the authors decided to extend previous research by exploring whether or not there is a correlation between “adolescents’ social media use and momentary affective well-being” (Beyens et al., 2020). The authors failed to correctly identify that this section was a literature review, making it harder for the reader to distinguish between the sections.

Regarding the methods, Beyens et al. (2020) used semi-random experience sampling (ESM) of 63 adolescents with an average age of 15 years old and identified as Dutch. All participants received informed consent from their parents, and the Ethics Review Board approved the study. The study procedure is divided into two phases: 1) a preliminary baseline study to gauge demographics and frequency of social media and 2) a 7-day ESM study personalized to each participant based on responses to the baseline survey. The ESM survey was used to analyze the social media use of the participants’ top three platforms (i.e., WhatsApp, Instagram, Snapchat, YouTube, and other chat functions of games). Additionally, the duration of social media use was measured to determine how much time “in the past hour they had spent actively and passively using” (Beyens et al., 2020) the three top platforms reported. Lastly, the researchers also included a line item to gauge participants’ affective well-being, asking about happiness levels on a 7-point scale. Overall, 2,155 assessments were collected, and an average of 34.83 (SD=4.91) surveys were completed.

The study results concluded that associations of passive social media use with well-being differ significantly from adolescent to adolescent, with 45.9% feeling better and only 9.84% feeling worse after passive use. Additionally, for Instagram, 73.91% of participants did not feel better or worse with passive social media use. Results also revealed that most adolescents did not experience any momentary changes in well-being related to their social media use. Nevertheless, the changes seen in the results on well-being are more positive than negative. Finally, Beyens et al. (2020) found that active and passive use have different effects on affective well-being. However, passive use of Instagram and WhatsApp yields more happiness than bad feelings.

Save Time On Research and Writing
Hire a Pro to Write You a 100% Plagiarism-Free Paper.
Get My Paper

Interestingly, the participants reported having no difference in well-being or a positive effect with passive social media use compared to active ones. In other journals I have read on this topic, passive social media use has contributed to negative well-being (i.e., anxiety and low self-esteem). However, the fact that this study was only a week-long might be why the results were so positive since the analyses only examined a short period of time.

One of the implications is that social media use was overall positive because adolescents often seek positive content like memes (Beyens et al., 2020). Lastly, they established a few limitations and areas for future research. Beyens et al. (2020) identified a possible limitation: the participants all seemed to be relatively happy. They proposed that future research be done with more vulnerable samples of adolescents with lower social-emotional well-being. Additionally, they recognized that the week-long study is only a brief overview of adolescent development. They added that future research would help to see if social media use with momentary affective well-being could turn into long-lasting consequences.

Please respond to at least one of the following questions and remember to cite at least one example/excerpt from the study in each response:

  1. The researchers mentioned they decided to focus on middle adolescents because they are more sensitive to reactions from peers and often compare themselves to others on social media. Do you agree with this statement? How might the results be different if the researchers focused on 17 and 18-year-olds as opposed to 14 and 15-year-olds?
  2. The participants reported that their top three platforms were Instagram, WhatsApp, and Snapchat. Are you surprised by the selection of platforms, and how might other platforms like Twitter, TikTok, or BeReal have an effect on the well-being of social media use for this age group?
  3. The majority of participants either have no effect on well-being or positive well-being due to passive social media use. Would the participants have more negative well-being if the study was longer? Also, does active social media use evoke more negative well-being compared to passive social media use? Why or why not?

www.nature.com/scientificreports
OPEN
The effect of social media
on well‑being differs
from adolescent to adolescent
Ine Beyens 1*, J. Loes Pouwels
Patti M. Valkenburg 1
1
, Irene I. van Driel
1
, Loes Keijsers
2
&
The question whether social media use benefits or undermines adolescents’ well-being is an important
societal concern. Previous empirical studies have mostly established across-the-board effects among
(sub)populations of adolescents. As a result, it is still an open question whether the effects are unique
for each individual adolescent. We sampled adolescents’ experiences six times per day for one week to
quantify differences in their susceptibility to the effects of social media on their momentary affective
well-being. Rigorous analyses of 2,155 real-time assessments showed that the association between
social media use and affective well-being differs strongly across adolescents: While 44% did not
feel better or worse after passive social media use, 46% felt better, and 10% felt worse. Our results
imply that person-specific effects can no longer be ignored in research, as well as in prevention and
intervention programs.
Ever since the introduction of social media, such as Facebook and Instagram, researchers have been studying
whether the use of such media may affect adolescents’ well-being. These studies have typically reported mixed
findings, yielding either small negative, small positive, or no effects of the time spent using social media on
different indicators of well-being, such as life satisfaction and depressive symptoms (for recent reviews, see for
­example1–5). Most of these studies have focused on between-person associations, examining whether adolescents
who use social media more (or less) often than their peers experience lower (or higher) levels of well-being than
these peers. While such between-person studies are valuable in their own right, several ­scholars6, 7 have recently
called for studies that investigate within-person associations to understand whether an increase in an adolescent’s
social media use is associated with an increase or decrease in that adolescent’s well-being. The current study
aims to respond to this call by investigating associations between social media use and well-being within single
adolescents across multiple points in t­ ime8–10.
Person‑specific effects. To our knowledge, four recent studies have investigated within-person associations of social media use with different indicators of adolescent well-being (i.e., life satisfaction, depression),
again with mixed r­ esults6, 11–13. Orben and ­colleagues6 found a small negative reciprocal within-person association between the time spent using social media and life satisfaction. Likewise, Boers and ­colleagues12 found a
small within-person association between social media use and increased depressive symptoms. Finally, Coyne
and ­colleagues11 and Jensen and ­colleagues13 did not find any evidence for within-person associations between
social media use and depression.
Earlier studies that investigated within-person associations of social media use with indicators of well-being
have all only reported average effect sizes. However, it is possible, or even plausible, that these average withinperson effects may have been small and nonsignificant because they result from sizeable heterogeneity in adolescents’ susceptibility to the effects of social media use on well-being ­(see14, 15). After all, an average within-person
effect size can be considered an aggregate of numerous individual within-person effect sizes that range from
highly positive to highly negative.
Some within-person studies have sought to understand adolescents’ differential susceptibility to the effects of
social media by investigating differences between subgroups. For instance, they have investigated the moderating role of sex to compare the effects of social media on boys versus g­ irls6, 11. However, such a group-differential
1
Amsterdam School of Communication Research, University of Amsterdam, 1001 NG Amsterdam,
The Netherlands. 2Department of Developmental Psychology, Tilburg University, 5000 LE Tilburg, The
Netherlands. *email: i.beyens@uva.nl
Scientific Reports |
(2020) 10:10763
| https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7
1
Vol.:(0123456789)
www.nature.com/scientificreports/
approach, in which potential differences in susceptibility are conceptualized by group-level moderators (e.g.,
gender, age) does not provide insights into more fine-grained differences at the level of the single i­ ndividual16.
After all, while girls and boys each represent a homogenous group in terms of sex, they may each differ on a
wide array of other factors.
As such, although worthwhile, the average within-person effects of social media on well-being obtained in
previous studies may have been small or non-significant because they are diluted across a highly heterogeneous
population (or sub-population) of ­adolescents14, 15. In line with the proposition of media effects theories that
each adolescent may have a unique susceptibility to the effects of social m
­ edia17, a viable explanation for the
small and inconsistent findings in earlier studies may be that the effect of social media differs from adolescent
to adolescent. The aim of the current study is to investigate this hypothesis and to obtain a better understanding
of adolescents’ unique susceptibility to the effects of social media on their affective well-being.
Social media and affective well‑being. Within-person studies have provided important insights into
the associations of social media use with cognitive well-being (e.g., life ­satisfaction6), which refers to adolescents’
cognitive judgment of how satisfied they are with their ­life18. However, the associations of social media use with
adolescents’ affective well-being (i.e., adolescents’ affective evaluations of their moods and ­emotions18) are still
unknown. In addition, while earlier within-person studies have focused on associations with trait-like conceptualizations of well-being11–13, that is, adolescents’ average well-being across specific time ­periods18, there is a
lack of studies that focus on well-being as a momentary affective state. Therefore, we extend previous research by
examining the association between adolescents’ social media use and their momentary affective well-being. Like
earlier experience sampling (ESM) studies among ­adults19, 20, we measured adolescents’ momentary affective
well-being with a single item. Adolescents’ momentary affective well-being was defined as their current feelings
of happiness, a commonly used question to measure well-being21, 22, which has high convergent validity, as evidenced by the strong correlations with the presence of positive affect and absence of negative affect.
To assess adolescents’ momentary affective well-being (henceforth referred to as well-being), we conducted
a week-long ESM study among 63 middle adolescents ages 14 and 15. Six times a day, adolescents were asked to
complete a survey using their own mobile phone, covering 42 assessments per adolescent, assessing their affective
well-being and social media use. In total, adolescents completed 2,155 assessments (83.2% average compliance).
We focused on middle adolescence, since this is the period in life characterized by most significant fluctuations in well-being23, 24. Also, in comparison to early and late adolescents, middle adolescents are more sensitive
to reactions from peers and have a strong tendency to compare themselves with others on social media and
beyond. Because middle adolescents typically use different social media platforms, in a complementary w
­ ay25–27,
each adolescent reported on his/her use of the three social media platforms that s/he used most frequently out of
the five most popular social media platforms among adolescents: WhatsApp, followed by Instagram, Snapchat,
YouTube, and, finally, the chat function of ­games28. In addition to investigating the association between overall
social media use and well-being (i.e., the summed use of adolescents’ three most frequently used platforms), we
examined the unique associations of the two most popular platforms, WhatsApp and ­Instagram28.
Like previous studies on social media use and well-being, we distinguished between active social media use
(i.e., “activities that facilitate direct exchanges with others”29) and passive social media use (i.e., “consuming
information without direct exchanges”29). Within-person studies among young adults have shown that passive
but not active social media use predicts decreases in well-being29. Therefore, we examined the unique associations
of adolescents’ overall active and passive social media use with their well-being, as well as active and passive use
of Instagram and WhatsApp, specifically. We investigated categorical associations, that is, whether adolescents
would feel better or worse if they had actively or passively used social media. And we investigated dose–response
associations to understand whether adolescents’ well-being would change as a function of the time they had
spent actively or passively using social media.
The hypotheses and the design, sampling and analysis plan were preregistered prior to data collection and
are available on the Open Science Framework, along with the code used in the analyses (https​://osf.io/nhks2​).
For details about the design of the study and analysis approach, see Methods.
Results
In more than half of all assessments (68.17%), adolescents had used social media (i.e., one or more of their three
favorite social media platforms), either in an active or passive way. Instagram (50.90%) and WhatsApp (53.52%)
were used in half of all assessments. Passive use of social media (66.21% of all assessments) was more common
than active use (50.86%), both on Instagram (48.48% vs. 20.79%) and WhatsApp (51.25% vs. 40.07%).
Strong positive between-person correlations were found between the duration of active and passive social
media use (overall: r = 0.69, p < 0.001; Instagram: r = 0.38, p < 0.01; WhatsApp: r = 0.85, p < 0.001): Adolescents who had spent more time actively using social media than their peers, had also spent more time passively using social media than their peers. Likewise, strong positive within-person correlations were found between the duration of active and passive social media use (overall: r = 0.63, p < 0.001; Instagram: r = 0.37, p < 0.001; WhatsApp: r = 0.57, p < 0.001): The more time an adolescent had spent actively using social media at a certain moment, the more time s/he had also spent passively using social media at that moment. Table 1 displays the average number of minutes that adolescents had spent using social media in the past hour at each assessment, and the zero-order between- and within-person correlations between the duration of social media use and well-being. At the between-person level, the duration of active and passive social media use was not associated with well-being: Adolescents who had spent more time actively or passively using social media than their peers did not report significantly higher or lower levels of well-being than their peers. At the within-person level, significant but weak positive correlations were found between the duration of active and Scientific Reports | Vol:.(1234567890) (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 2 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ M (SD) Well-being between-person Well-being within-person Well-being 5.61 (0.75) – Duration of overall active social media use 12.47 (11.49) .06 – .09** Duration of overall passive social media use 19.71 (8.95) .17 .07* Duration of active Instagram use 5.15 (6.69) − .03 .04 Duration of passive Instagram use 9.39 (4.84) − .07 .03 Duration of active WhatsApp use 5.34 (4.14) .08 .04 Duration of passive WhatsApp use 7.34 (3.69) .01 .09*** Table 1.  Means, standard deviations, and zero-order between-person and within-person correlations for well-being and duration of use. Means were calculated at the between-person level and represent the average number of minutes that adolescents had spent using social media in the past hour across assessments during which adolescents had used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp in the past hour. All correlations are based on the assessments during which participants had used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp, either actively or passively. N = 63 for active and passive overall social media use; N = 60 for active and passive Instagram use; N = 63 for active and passive WhatsApp use. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Categorical associations Use versus no use (N participants = 63; N assessments = 2,155) Dose–response associations Duration of use (N participants = 63; N assessments = 1,474) Model 1A Fixed effects Model 2A Fixed effects B (SE) Model 1B Random effects p β B (SE) p B (SE) Model 2B Random effects p β B (SE) p Fixed part Intercept 5.60 (.10) < .001 7.63 5.60 (.10) < .001 5.66 (.09) < .001 8.05 5.66 (.09) < .001 Assessment (WP) .09 (.04) .020 .09 .09 (.04) .015 .07 (.04) .094 .07 .06 (.04) .091 Passive use (WP) .14 (.09) .111 .06 .14 (.09) .114 .03 (.03) .270 .04 .03 (.03) .243 Active use (WP) .14 (.08) .096 .05 .15 (.08) .058 .07 (.04) .040 .07 .07 (.04) .053 σ2 residual (WP) 1.16 (.11) < .001 1.13 (.11) < .001 1.10 (.11) < .001 1.08 (.11) < .001 σ2 between-person (BP) .54 (.08) < .001 .54 (.08) < .001 .49 (.09) < .001 .50 (.09) < .001 σ2 passive use (BP) .11 (.05) .015 < .01 (.01) .333 σ2 active use (BP) .05 (.06) .209 .01 (.01) .221 Random part Model fit Deviance 6,616.58 6,603.86 4,470.03 4,467.78 AIC 6,628.58 6,619.86 4,482.03 4,483.78 BIC 6,662.63 6,665.26 4,513.80 4,526.14 Chi2 (df) 17.52 (2) < .001 2.62 (2) .270 Table 2.  Within-person associations between adolescents’ overall use of social media and well-being. For investigating the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive use: 0 = no passive use of social media; 1 = passive use of social media; and active use: 0 = no active use of social media; 1 = active use of social media, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All predictors were person-mean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which participants had used social media, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are twosided, p-values of the random part of the model are one-sided. passive overall social media use and well-being. This indicates that adolescents felt somewhat better at moments when they had spent more time actively or passively using social media (overall), compared to moments when they had spent less time actively or passively using social media. When looking at specific platforms, a positive correlation was only found for passive WhatsApp use, but not for active WhatsApp use, and not for active and passive Instagram use. Average and person‑specific effects. The within-person associations of social media use with wellbeing and differences in these associations were tested in a series of multilevel models. We ran separate models for overall social media use (i.e., active use and passive use of adolescents’ three favorite social media platforms, see Table 2), Instagram use (see Table 3), and WhatsApp use (see Table 4). In a first step we examined the average categorical associations for each of these three social media uses using fixed effects models (Models 1A, 3A, and 5A) to investigate whether, on average, adolescents would feel better or worse at moments when they had Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 3 Vol.:(0123456789) www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Categorical associations Use versus no use (N participants = 60; N assessments = 2,112) Dose–response associations Duration of use (N participants = 60; N assessments = 1,075) Model 3A Fixed effects Model 4A Fixed effects B (SE) Model 3B Random effects p β B (SE) p B (SE) Model 4B Random effects p β B (SE) p Fixed part Intercept 5.64 (.10) < .001 7.89 5.64 (.10) < .001 5.69 (.10) < .001 7.62 5.69 (.10) < .001 Assessment (WP) .08 (.04) .031 .09 .08 (.04) .033 .05 (.05) .347 .09 .05 (.05) .276 Passive use (WP) .15 (.07) .027 .14 .15 (.07) .041 .03 (.07) .695 .14 .06 (.07) .440 Active use (WP) .14 (.09) .137 .13 .13 (.09) .164 .05 (.06) .428 .06 .01 (.07) .879 σ2 residual (WP) 1.18 (.11) < .001 1.17 (.10) < .001 1.11 (.12) < .001 1.07 (.12) < .001 σ2 between-person (BP) .54 (.08) < .001 .54 (.08) < .001 .47 (.08) < .001 .47 (.08) < .001 σ2 passive use (BP) .02 (.05) .335 .06 (.04) .068 σ2 active use (BP) .05 (.07) .217 .04 (.04) .142 Random part Model fit Deviance 6,503.54 6,501.67 3,287.59 3,279.66 AIC 6,515.54 6,517.67 3,299.59 3,295.33 BIC 6,549.48 6,562.91 Chi2 (df) 3,329.47 1.37 (2) 3,335.17 .503 19.92 (2) < .001 Table 3.  Within-person associations between adolescents’ use of Instagram and well-being. For investigating the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive use: 0 = no passive use of Instagram; 1 = passive use of Instagram; and active use: 0 = no active use of Instagram; 1 = active use of Instagram, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All predictors were personmean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which participants had used Instagram, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are two-sided, p-values of the random part of the model are one-sided. Categorical associations Use versus no use (N participants = 63; N assessments = 2,154) Dose–response associations Duration of use (N participants = 62; N assessments = 1,179) Model 5A Fixed effects Model 6A Fixed effects B (SE) Intercept 5.60 Assessment (WP) .08 Passive use (WP) Active use (WP) Model 5B Random effects p β B (SE) p B (SE) (.10) < .001 7.62 5.60 (.04) .023 .09 .09 (.11) < .001 5.69 (.04) .019 .09 .16 (.05) .001 .14 .06 (.07) .364 .06 .22 (.09) .022 .18 .02 (.09) .810 − .05 σ2 residual (WP) 1.16 (.11) σ2 between-person (BP) .54 (.08) < .001 1.15 (.11) < .001 < .001 .54 (.08) < .001 σ2 passive use (BP) .04 (.08) σ2 active use (BP) .02 (.05) Model 6B Random effects p β B (SE) p (.09) < .001 7.62 5.69 (.09) < .001 (.04) .049 .09 .09 (.04) .047 (.05) < .001 .14 .18 (.04) < .001 (.06) .444 .06 − .05 (.05) .258 1.10 (.11) < .001 1.10 (.12) < .001 .47 (.09) < .001 .47 (.09) < .001 .296 < .01 (.01) .440 .360 < .01 (.01) .310 Fixed part Random part Model fit Deviance 6,615.24 6,613.47 3,590.86 3,591.40 AIC 6,627.24 6,629.47 3,602.86 3,607.40 BIC 6,661.29 6,674.87 3,633.30 3,647.98 Chi2 (df) 1.14 (2) .566 2.81 (2) .245 Table 4.  Within-person associations between adolescents’ use of WhatsApp and well-being. For investigating the categorical associations, the passive and active use predictors were dummy coded (passive use: 0 = no passive use of WhatsApp; 1 = passive use of WhatsApp; and active use: 0 = no active use of WhatsApp; 1 = active use of WhatsApp, respectively). WP = within-person; BP = between-person. All predictors were personmean centered. Models for the duration of use only include assessments during which participants had used WhatsApp, either actively or passively. p-values of the fixed part of the model are two-sided, p-values of the random part of the model are one-sided. Scientific Reports | Vol:.(1234567890) (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 4 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ used social media compared to moments when they had not (i.e., categorical predictors: active use versus no active use, and passive use versus no passive use). In a second step, we examined heterogeneity in the withinperson categorical associations by adding random slopes to the fixed effects models (Models 1B, 3B, and 5B). Next, we examined the average dose–response associations using fixed effects models (Models 2A, 4A, and 6A), to investigate whether, on average, adolescents would feel better or worse when they had spent more time using social media (i.e., continuous predictors: duration of active use and duration of passive use). Finally, we examined heterogeneity in the within-person dose–response associations by adding random slopes to the fixed effects models (Models 2B, 4B, and 6B). Overall social media use.. The model with the categorical predictors (see Table 2; Model 1A) showed that, on average, there was no association between overall use and well-being: Adolescents’ well-being did not increase or decrease at moments when they had used social media, either in a passive or active way. However, evidence was found that the association of passive (but not active) social media use with well-being differed from adolescent to adolescent (Model 1B), with effect sizes ranging from − 0.24 to 0.68. For 44.26% of the adolescents the association was non-existent to small (− 0.10 < r < 0.10). However, for 45.90% of the adolescents there was a weak (0.10 < r < 0.20; 8.20%), moderate (0.20 < r < 0.30; 22.95%) or even strong positive (r ≥ 0.30; 14.75%) association between overall passive social media use and well-being, and for almost one in ten (9.84%) adolescents there was a weak (− 0.20 < r < − 0.10; 6.56%) or moderate negative (− 0.30 < r < − 0.20; 3.28%) association. The model with continuous predictors (Model 2A) showed that, on average, there was a significant dose–response association for active use. At moments when adolescents had used social media, the time they spent actively (but not passively) using social media was positively associated with well-being: Adolescents felt better at moments when they had spent more time sending messages, posting, or sharing something on social media. The associations of the time spent actively and passively using social media with well-being did not differ across adolescents (Model 2B). Instagram use. As shown in Model 3A in Table 3, on average, there was a significant categorical association between passive (but not active) Instagram use and well-being: Adolescents experienced an increase in wellbeing at moments when they had passively used Instagram (i.e., viewing posts/stories of others). Adolescents did not experience an increase or decrease in well-being when they had actively used Instagram. The associations of passive and active Instagram use with well-being did not differ across adolescents (Model 3B). On average, no significant dose–response association was found for Instagram use (Model 4A): At moments when adolescents had used Instagram, the time adolescents spent using Instagram (either actively or passively) was not associated with their well-being. However, evidence was found that the association of the time spent passively using Instagram differed from adolescent to adolescent (Model 4B), with effect sizes ranging from − 0.48 to 0.27. For most adolescents (73.91%) the association was non-existent to small (− 0.10 < r < 0.10), but for almost one in five adolescents (17.39%) there was a weak (0.10 < r < 0.20; 10.87%) or moderate (0.20 < r < 0.30; 6.52%) positive association, and for almost one in ten adolescents (8.70%) there was a weak (− 0.20 < r < − 0.10; 2.17%), moderate (− 0.30 < r < − 0.20; 4.35%), or strong (r ≤ − 0.30; 2.17%) negative association. Figure 1 illustrates these differences in the dose–response associations. WhatsApp use. As shown in Model 5A in Table 4, just as for Instagram, we found that, on average, there was a significant categorical association between passive (but not active) WhatsApp use and well-being: Adolescents reported that they felt better at moments when they had passively used WhatsApp (i.e., read WhatsApp messages). For active WhatsApp use, no significant association was found. Also, in line with the results for Instagram use, no differences were found regarding the associations of active and passive WhatsApp use (Model 5B). In addition, a significant dose–response association was found for passive (but not active) use (Model 6A). At moments when adolescents had used WhatsApp, we found that, on average, the time adolescents spent passively using WhatsApp was positively associated with well-being: Adolescents felt better at moments when they had spent more time reading WhatsApp messages. The time spent actively using WhatsApp was not associated with well-being. No differences were found in the dose–response associations of active and passive WhatsApp use (Model 6B). Discussion This preregistered study investigated adolescents’ unique susceptibility to the effects of social media. We found that the associations of passive (but not active) social media use with well-being differed substantially from adolescent to adolescent, with effect sizes ranging from moderately negative (− 0.24) to strongly positive (0.68). While 44.26% of adolescents did not feel better or worse if they had passively used social media, 45.90% felt better, and a small group felt worse (9.84%). In addition, for Instagram the majority of adolescents (73.91%) did not feel better or worse when they had spent more time viewing post or stories of others, whereas some felt better (17.39%), and others (8.70%) felt worse. These findings have important implications for social media effects research, and media effects research more generally. For decades, researchers have argued that people differ in their susceptibility to the effects of ­media17, leading to numerous investigations of such differential susceptibility. These investigations have typically focused on moderators, based on variables such as sex, age, or personality. Yet, over the years, studies have shown that such moderators appear to have little power to explain how individuals differ in their susceptibility to media effects, probably because a group-differential approach does not account for the possibility that media users may differ across a range of factors, that are not captured by only one (or a few) investigated moderator variables. Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 5 Vol.:(0123456789) www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 1.  The dose–response association between passive Instagram use (in minutes per hour) and affective well-being for each individual adolescent (n = 46). Red lines represent significant negative within-person associations, green lines represent significant positive within-person associations, and gray lines represent nonsignificant within-person associations. A graph was created for each participant who had completed at least 10 assessments. A total of 13 participants were excluded because they had completed less than 10 assessments of passive Instagram use. In addition, one participant was excluded because no graph could be computed, since this participant’s passive Instagram use was constant across assessments. By providing insights into each individual’s unique susceptibility, the findings of this study provide an explanation as to why, up until now, most media effects research has only found small effects. We found that the majority of adolescents do not experience any short-term changes in well-being related to their social media use. And if they do experience any changes, these are more often positive than negative. Because only small subsets of adolescents experience small to moderate changes in well-being, the true effects of social media reported in previous studies have probably been diluted across heterogeneous samples of individuals that differ in their susceptibility to media effects (also s­ ee30). Several scholars have noted that overall effect sizes may mask more subtle individual ­differences14, 15, which may explain why previous studies have typically reported small or no effects of social media on well-being or indicators of well-being6, 11–13. The current study seems to confirm this assumption, by showing that while the overall effect sizes are small at best, the person-specific effect sizes vary considerably, from tiny and small to moderate and strong. As called upon by other ­scholars5, 31, we disentangled the associations of active and passive use of social media. Research among young adults found that passive (but not active) social media use is associated with lower levels of affective well-being29. In line with these findings, the current study shows that active and passive use yielded different associations with adolescents’ affective well-being. Interestingly though, in contrast to previous findings among adults, our study showed that, on average, passive use of Instagram and WhatsApp seemed to enhance rather than decrease adolescents’ well-being. This discrepancy in findings may be attributed to the fact that different mechanisms might be involved. Verduyn and ­colleagues29 found that passive use of Facebook undermines adults’ well-being by enhancing envy, which may also explain the decreases in well-being found in our study among a small group of adolescents. Yet, adolescents who felt better by passively using Instagram and WhatsApp, might have felt so because they experienced enjoyment. After all, adolescents often seek positive content on social media, such as humorous posts or ­memes32. Also, research has shown that adolescents mainly receive positive feedback on social m ­ edia33. Hence, their passive Instagram and WhatsApp use may involve the reading of positive feedback, which may explain the increases in well-being. Overall, the time spent passively using WhatsApp improved adolescents’ well-being. This did not differ from adolescent to adolescent. However, the associations of the time spent passively using Instagram with well-being did differ from adolescent to adolescent. This discrepancy suggests that not all social media uses yield personspecific effects on well-being. A possible explanation may be that adolescents’ responses to WhatsApp are more homogenous than those to Instagram. WhatsApp is a more private platform, which is mostly used for one-toone communication with friends and a­ cquaintances26. Instagram, in contrast, is a more public platform, which Scientific Reports | Vol:.(1234567890) (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 6 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ allows its users to follow a diverse set of people, ranging from best friends to singers, actors, and i­ nfluencers28, and to engage in intimate communication as well as self-presentation and social comparison. Such diverse uses could lead to more varied, or even opposing responses, such as envy versus inspiration. Limitations and directions for future research. The current study extends our understanding of differential susceptibility to media effects, by revealing that the effect of social media use on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. The findings confirm our assumption that among the great majority of adolescents, social media use is unrelated to well-being, but that among a small subset, social media use is either related to decreases or increases in well-being. It must be noted, however, that participants in this study felt relatively happy, overall. Studies with more vulnerable samples, consisting of clinical samples or youth with lower socialemotional well-being may elicit different patterns of ­effects27. Also, the current study focused on affective wellbeing, operationalized as happiness. It is plausible that social media use relates differently with other types of well-being, such as cognitive well-being. An important next step is to identify which adolescents are particularly susceptible to experience declines in well-being. It is conceivable, for instance, that the few adolescents who feel worse when they use social media are the ones who receive negative feedback on social m ­ edia33. In addition, future ESM studies into the effects of social media should attempt to include one or more followup measures to improve our knowledge of the longer-term influence of social media use on affective well-being. While a week-long ESM is very common and applied in most earlier ESM s­ tudies34, a week is only a snapshot of adolescent development. Research is needed that investigates whether the associations of social media use with adolescents’ momentary affective well-being may cumulate into long-lasting consequences. Such investigations could help clarify whether adolescents who feel bad in the short term would experience more negative consequences in the long term, and whether adolescents who feel better would be more resistant to developing longterm negative consequences. And while most adolescents do not seem to experience any short-term increases or decreases in well-being, more research is needed to investigate whether these adolescents may experience a longer-term impact of social media. While the use of different platforms may be differently associated with well-being, different types of use may also yield different effects. Although the current study distinguished between active and passive use of social media, future research should further differentiate between different activities. For instance, because passive use entails many different activities, from reading private messages (e.g., WhatsApp messages, direct messages on Instagram) to browsing a public feed (e.g., scrolling through posts on Instagram), research is needed that explores the unique effects of passive public use and passive private use. Research that seeks to explore the nuances in adolescents’ susceptibility as well as the nuances in their social media use may truly improve our understanding of the effects of social media use. Methods Participants. Participants were recruited via a secondary school in the south of the Netherlands. Our preregistered sampling plan set a target sample size of 100 adolescents. We invited adolescents from six classrooms to participate in the study. The final sample consisted of 63 adolescents (i.e., 42% consent rate, which is comparable to other ESM studies among adolescents; see, for i­nstance35, 36). Informed consent was obtained from all participants and their parents. On average, participants were 15 years old (M = 15.12 years, SD = 0.51) and 54% were girls. All participants self-identified as Dutch, and 41.3% were enrolled in the prevocational secondary education track, 25.4% in the intermediate general secondary education track, and 33.3% in the academic preparatory education track. Procedure. The study was approved by the Ethics Review Board of the Faculty of Social and Behavioral Sciences at the University of Amsterdam and was performed in accordance with the guidelines formulated by the Ethics Review Board. The study consisted of two phases: A baseline survey and a personalized week-long experience sampling (ESM) study. In phase 1, researchers visited the school during school hours. Researchers informed the participants of the objective and procedure of the study and assured them that their responses would be treated confidentially. Participants were asked to sign the consent form. Next, participants completed a 15-min baseline survey. The baseline survey included questions about demographics and assessed which social media each adolescent used most frequently, allowing to personalize the social media questions presented during the ESM study in phase 2. After completing the baseline survey, participants were provided detailed instructions about phase 2. In phase 2, which took place two and a half weeks after the baseline survey, a 7-day ESM study was conducted, following the guidelines for ESM studies provided by van Roekel and c­ olleagues34. Aiming for at least 30 assessments per participant and based on an average compliance rate of 70 to 80% reported in earlier ESM studies among ­adolescents34, we asked each participant to complete a total of 42 ESM surveys (i.e., six 2-min surveys per day). Participants completed the surveys using their own mobile phone, on which the ESM software application Ethica Data was installed during the instruction session with the researchers (phase 1). Each 2-min survey consisted of 22 questions, which assessed adolescents’ well-being and social media use. Two open-ended questions were added to the final survey of the day, which asked about adolescents’ most pleasant and most unpleasant events of the day. The ESM sampling scheme was semi-random, to allow for randomization and avoid structural patterns in well-being, while taking into account that adolescents were not allowed to use their phone during school time. The Ethica Data app was programmed to generate six beep notifications per day at random time points within a fixed time interval that was tailored to the school’s schedule: before school time (1 beep), during school breaks (2 beeps), and after school time (3 beeps). During the weekend, the beeps were generated during the morning Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 7 Vol.:(0123456789) www.nature.com/scientificreports/ (1 beep), afternoon (3 beeps), and evening (2 beeps). To maximize compliance, a 30-min time window was provided to complete each survey. This time window was extended to one hour for the first survey (morning) and two hours for the final survey (evening) to account for travel time to school and time spent on evening activities. The average compliance rate was 83.2%. A total of 2,155 ESM assessments were collected: Participants completed an average of 34.83 surveys (SD = 4.91) on a total of 42 surveys, which is high compared to previous ESM studies among ­adolescents34. The questions of the ESM study were personalized based on the responses to the baseline survey. During the ESM study, each participant reported on his/her use of three different social media platforms: WhatsApp and either Instagram, Snapchat, YouTube, and/or the chat function of games (i.e., the most popular social media platforms among a­ dolescents28). Questions about Instagram and WhatsApp use were only included if the participant had indicated in the baseline survey that s/he used these platforms at least once a week. If a participant had indicated that s/he used Instagram or WhatsApp (or both) less than once a week, s/he was asked to report on the use of Snapchat, YouTube, or the chat function of games, depending on what platform s/he used at least once a week. In addition to Instagram and WhatsApp, questions were asked about a third platform, that was selected based on how frequently the participant used Snapchat, YouTube, or the chat function of games (i.e., at least once a week). This resulted in five different combinations of three platforms: Instagram, WhatsApp, and Snapchat (47 participants); Instagram, WhatsApp, and YouTube (11 participants); Instagram, WhatsApp, and chatting via games (2 participants); WhatsApp, Snapchat, and YouTube (1 participant); and WhatsApp, YouTube, and chatting via games (2 participants). Frequency of social media use. In the baseline survey, participants were asked to indicate how often they used and checked Instagram, WhatsApp, Snapchat, YouTube, and the chat function of games, using response options ranging from 1 (never) to 7 (more than 12 times per day). These platforms are the five most popular platforms among Dutch 14- and 15-year-olds28. Participants’ responses were used to select the three social media platforms that were assessed in the personalized ESM study. Duration of social media use. In the ESM study, duration of active and passive social media use was measured by asking participants how much time in the past hour they had spent actively and passively using each of the three platforms that were included in the personalized ESM surveys. Response options ranged from 0 to 60 min, with 5-min intervals. To measure active Instagram use, participants indicated how much time in the past hour they had spent (a) “posting on your feed or sharing something in your story on Instagram” and (b) “sending direct messages/chatting on Instagram.” These two items were summed to create the variable duration of active Instagram use. Sum scores exceeding 60 min (only 0.52% of all assessments) were recoded to 60 min. To measure duration of passive Instagram use, participants indicated how much time in the past hour they had spent “viewing posts/stories of others on Instagram.” To measure the use of WhatsApp, Snapchat, YouTube and game-based chatting, we asked participants how much time they had spent “sending WhatsApp messages” (active use) and “reading WhatsApp messages” (passive use); “sending snaps/messages or sharing something in your story on Snapchat” (active use) and “viewing snaps/stories/messages from others on Snapchat” (passive use); “posting YouTube clips” (active use) and “watching YouTube clips” (passive use); “sending messages via the chat function of a game/games” (active use) and “reading messages via the chat function of a game/games” (passive use). Duration of active and passive overall social media use were created by summing the responses across the three social media platforms for active and passive use, respectively. Sum scores exceeding 60 min (2.13% of all assessments for active overall use; 2.90% for passive overall use) were recoded to 60 min. The duration variables were used to investigate whether the time spent actively or passively using social media was associated with well-being (dose–response associations). Use/no use of social media. Based on the duration variables, we created six dummy variables, one for active and one for passive overall social media use, one for active and one for passive Instagram use, and one for active and one for passive WhatsApp use (0 = no active use and 1 = active use, and 0 = no passive use and 1 = passive use, respectively). These dummy variables were used to investigate whether the use of social media, irrespective of the duration of use, was associated with well-being (categorical associations). Well‑being. Consistent with previous ESM s­ tudies19, 20, we measured affective well-being using one item, asking “How happy do you feel right now?” at each assessment. Adolescents indicated their response to the question using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 7 (completely), with 4 (a little) as the midpoint. Convergent validity of this item was established in a separate pilot ESM study among 30 adolescents conducted by the research team of the fourth author: The affective well-being item was strongly correlated with the presence of positive affect and absence of negative affect (assessed by a 10-item positive and negative affect schedule for children; PANAS-C) at both the between-person (positive affect: r = 0.88, p < 0.001; negative affect: r = − 0.62, p < 0.001) and within-person level (positive affect: r = 0.74, p < 0.001; negative affect: r = − 0.58, p < 0.001). Statistical analyses. Before conducting the analyses, several validation checks were performed ­(see34). First, we aimed to only include participants in the analyses who had completed more than 33% of all ESM assessments (i.e., at least 14 assessments). Next, we screened participants’ responses to the open questions for unserious responses (e.g., gross comments, jokes). And finally, we inspected time series plots for patterns in answering tendencies. Since all participants completed more than 33% of all ESM assessments, and no inappropriate responses or low-quality data patterns were detected, all participants were included in the analyses. Scientific Reports | Vol:.(1234567890) (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 8 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Following our preregistered analysis plan, we tested the proposed associations in a series of multilevel models. Before doing so, we tested the homoscedasticity and linearity assumptions for multilevel a­ nalyses37. Inspection of standardized residual plots indicated that the data met these assumptions (plots are available on OSF at https​ ://osf.io/nhks2​). We specified separate models for overall social media use, use of Instagram, and use of WhatsApp. To investigate to what extent adolescents’ well-being would vary depending on whether they had actively or passively used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp or not during the past hour (categorical associations), we tested models including the dummy variables as predictors (active use versus no active use, and passive use versus no passive use; models 1, 3, and 5). To investigate whether, at moments when adolescents had used social media/Instagram/WhatsApp during the past hour, their well-being would vary depending on the duration of social media/Instagram/WhatsApp use (dose–response associations), we tested models including the duration variables as predictors (duration of active use and duration of passive use; models 2, 4, and 6). In order to avoid negative skew in the duration variables, we only included assessments during which adolescents had used social media in the past hour (overall, Instagram, or WhatsApp, respectively), either actively or passively. All models included well-being as outcome variable. Since multilevel analyses allow to include all available data for each individual, no missing data were imputed and no data points were excluded. We used a model building approach that involved three steps. In the first step, we estimated an intercept-only model to assess the relative amount of between- and within-person variance in affective well-being. We estimated a three-level model in which repeated momentary assessments (level 1) were nested within adolescents (level 2), who, in turn, were nested within classrooms (level 3). However, because the between-classroom variance in affective well-being was small (i.e., 0.4% of the variance was explained by differences between classes), we proceeded with estimating two-level (instead of three-level) models, with repeated momentary assessments (level 1) nested within adolescents (level 2). In the second step, we assessed the within-person associations of well-being with (a) overall active and passive social media use (i.e., the total of the three platforms), (b) active and passive use of Instagram, and (c) active and passive use of WhatsApp, by adding fixed effects to the model (Models 1A-6A). To facilitate the interpretation of the associations and control for the effects of time, a covariate was added that controlled for the nth assessment of the study week (instead of the nth assessment of the day, as preregistered). This so-called detrending is helpful to interpret within-person associations as correlated fluctuations beyond other changes in social media use and well-being38. In order to obtain within-person estimates, we person-mean centered all ­predictors38. Significance of the fixed effects was determined using the Wald test. In the third and final step, we assessed heterogeneity in the within-person associations by adding random slopes to the models (Models 1B-6B). Significance of the random slopes was determined by comparing the fit of the fixed effects model with the fit of the random effects model, by performing the Satorra-Bentler scaled chi-square ­test39 and by comparing the Bayesian information criterion ­(BIC40) and Akaike information criterion ­(AIC41) of the models. When the random effects model had a significantly better fit than the fixed effects model (i.e., pointing at significant heterogeneity), variance components were inspected to investigate whether heterogeneity existed in the association of either active or passive use. Next, when evidence was found for significant heterogeneity, we computed person-specific effect sizes, based on the random effect models, to investigate what percentages of adolescents experienced better well-being, worse well-being, and no changes in well-being. In line with Keijsers and c­ olleagues42 we only included participants who had completed at least 10 assessments. In addition, for the dose–response associations, we constructed graphical representations of the person-specific slopes, based on the person-specific effect sizes, using the xyplot function from the lattice package in ­R43. Three improvements were made to our original preregistered plan. First, rather than estimating the models with multilevel modelling in R ­ 43, we ran the preregistered models in M ­ plus44. Mplus provides standardized estimates for the fixed effects models, which offers insight into the effect sizes. This allowed us to compare the relative strength of the associations of passive versus active use with well-being. Second, instead of using the maximum likelihood estimator, we used the maximum likelihood estimator with robust standard errors (MLR), which are robust to non-normality. Sensitivity tests, uploaded on OSF (https​://osf.io/nhks2​), indicated that the results were almost identical across the two software packages and estimation approaches. Third, to improve the interpretation of the results and make the scales of the duration measures of social media use and well-being more comparable, we transformed the social media duration scores (0 to 60 min) into scales running from 0 to 6, so that an increase of 1 unit reflects 10 min of social media use. The model estimates were unaffected by this transformation. Reporting summary. Further information on the research design is available in the Nature Research Reporting Summary linked to this article. Data availability The dataset generated and analysed during the current study is available in F ­ igshare45. The preregistration of the design, sampling and analysis plan, and the analysis scripts used to analyse the data for this paper are available online on the Open Science Framework website (https​://osf.io/nhks2​). Received: 24 January 2020; Accepted: 11 June 2020 References 1. Best, P., Manktelow, R. & Taylor, B. Online communication, social media and adolescent wellbeing: A systematic narrative review. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 41, 27–36. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.child​youth​.2014.03.001 (2014). Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 9 Vol.:(0123456789) www.nature.com/scientificreports/ 2. James, C. et al. Digital life and youth well-being, social connectedness, empathy, and narcissism. Pediatrics 140, S71–S75. https​:// doi.org/10.1542/peds.2016-1758F​(2017). 3. McCrae, N., Gettings, S. & Purssell, E. Social media and depressive symptoms in childhood and adolescence: A systematic review. Adolesc. Res. Rev. 2, 315–330. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s4089​4-017-0053-4 (2017). 4. Sarmiento, I. G. et al. How does social media use relate to adolescents’ internalizing symptoms? Conclusions from a systematic narrative review. Adolesc Res Rev, 1–24, doi:10.1007/s40894-018-0095-2 (2018). 5. Orben, A. Teenagers, screens and social media: A narrative review of reviews and key studies. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. https​://doi.org/10.1007/s0012​7-019-01825​-4 (2020). 6. Orben, A., Dienlin, T. & Przybylski, A. K. Social media’s enduring effect on adolescent life satisfaction. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 116, 10226–10228. https​://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.19020​58116​ (2019). 7. Whitlock, J. & Masur, P. K. Disentangling the association of screen time with developmental outcomes and well-being: Problems, challenges, and opportunities. JAMA https​://doi.org/10.1001/jamap​ediat​rics.2019.3191 (2019). 8. Hamaker, E. L. In Handbook of Research Methods for Studying Daily Life (eds Mehl, M. R. & Conner, T. S.) 43–61 (Guilford Press, New York, 2012). 9. Schmiedek, F. & Dirk, J. In The Encyclopedia of Adulthood and Aging (ed. Krauss Whitbourne, S.) 1–6 (Wiley, 2015). 10. Keijsers, L. & van Roekel, E. In Reframing Adolescent Research (eds Hendry, L. B. & Kloep, M.) (Routledge, 2018). 11. Coyne, S. M., Rogers, A. A., Zurcher, J. D., Stockdale, L. & Booth, M. Does time spent using social media impact mental health? An eight year longitudinal study. Comput. Hum. Behav. 104, 106160. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2019.10616​0 (2020). 12. Boers, E., Afzali, M. H., Newton, N. & Conrod, P. Association of screen time and depression in adolescence. JAMA 173, 853–859. https​://doi.org/10.1001/jamap​ediat​rics.2019.1759 (2019). 13. Jensen, M., George, M. J., Russell, M. R. & Odgers, C. L. Young adolescents’ digital technology use and mental health symptoms: Little evidence of longitudinal or daily linkages. Clin. Psychol. Sci. https​://doi.org/10.1177/21677​02619​85933​6 (2019). 14. Valkenburg, P. M. The limited informativeness of meta-analyses of media effects. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 680–682. https​://doi. org/10.1177/17456​91615​59223​7 (2015). 15. Pearce, L. J. & Field, A. P. The impact of “scary” TV and film on children’s internalizing emotions: A meta-analysis. Hum. Commun.. Res. 42, 98–121. https​://doi.org/10.1111/hcre.12069​ (2016). 16. Howard, M. C. & Hoffman, M. E. Variable-centered, person-centered, and person-specific approaches. Organ. Res. Methods 21, 846–876. https​://doi.org/10.1177/10944​28117​74402​1 (2017). 17. Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. The differential susceptibility to media effects model. J. Commun. 63, 221–243. https:​ //doi.org/10.1111/ jcom.12024​(2013). 18. Eid, M. & Diener, E. Global judgments of subjective well-being: Situational variability and long-term stability. Soc. Indic. Res. 65, 245–277. https​://doi.org/10.1023/B:SOCI.00000​03801​.89195​.bc (2004). 19. Kross, E. et al. Facebook use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS ONE 8, e69841. https:​ //doi.org/10.1371/ journ​al.pone.00698​41 (2013). 20. Reissmann, A., Hauser, J., Stollberg, E., Kaunzinger, I. & Lange, K. W. The role of loneliness in emerging adults’ everyday use of facebook—An experience sampling approach. Comput. Hum. Behav. 88, 47–60. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2018.06.011 (2018). 21. Rutledge, R. B., Skandali, N., Dayan, P. & Dolan, R. J. A computational and neural model of momentary subjective well-being. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 111, 12252–12257. https​://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.14075​35111​ (2014). 22. Tov, W. In Handbook of Well-being (eds Diener, E.D. et al.) (DEF Publishers, 2018). 23. Harter, S. The Construction of the Self: Developmental and Sociocultural Foundations (Guilford Press, New York, 2012). 24. Steinberg, L. Adolescence. Vol. 9 (McGraw-Hill, 2011). 25. Rideout, V. & Fox, S. Digital Health Practices, Social Media Use, and Mental Well-being Among Teens and Young Adults in the US (HopeLab, San Francisco, 2018). 26. Waterloo, S. F., Baumgartner, S. E., Peter, J. & Valkenburg, P. M. Norms of online expressions of emotion: Comparing Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, and WhatsApp. New Media Soc. 20, 1813–1831. https​://doi.org/10.1177/14614​44817​70734​9 (2017). 27. Rideout, V. & Robb, M. B. Social Media, Social Life: Teens Reveal their Experiences (Common Sense Media, San Fransico, 2018). 28. van Driel, I. I., Pouwels, J. L., Beyens, I., Keijsers, L. & Valkenburg, P. M. ’Posting, Scrolling, Chatting & Snapping’: Youth (14–15) and Social Media in 2019 (Center for Research on Children, Adolescents, and the Media (CcaM), Universiteit van Amsterdam, 2019). 29. Verduyn, P. et al. Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence. J. Exp. Psychol. 144, 480–488. https​://doi.org/10.1037/xge00​00057​ (2015). 30. Valkenburg, P. M. & Peter, J. Five challenges for the future of media-effects research. Int. J. Commun. 7, 197–215 (2013). 31. Verduyn, P., Ybarra, O., Résibois, M., Jonides, J. & Kross, E. Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review. Soc. Issues Policy Rev. 11, 274–302. https​://doi.org/10.1111/sipr.12033​ (2017). 32. Radovic, A., Gmelin, T., Stein, B. D. & Miller, E. Depressed adolescents’ positive and negative use of social media. J. Adolesc. 55, 5–15. https​://doi.org/10.1016/j.adole​scenc​e.2016.12.002 (2017). 33. Valkenburg, P. M., Peter, J. & Schouten, A. P. Friend networking sites and their relationship to adolescents’ well-being and social self-esteem. Cyberpsychol. Behav. 9, 584–590. https​://doi.org/10.1089/cpb.2006.9.584 (2006). 34. van Roekel, E., Keijsers, L. & Chung, J. M. A review of current ambulatory assessment studies in adolescent samples and practical recommendations. J. Res. Adolesc. 29, 560–577. https​://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12471​ (2019). 35. van Roekel, E., Scholte, R. H. J., Engels, R. C. M. E., Goossens, L. & Verhagen, M. Loneliness in the daily lives of adolescents: An experience sampling study examining the effects of social contexts. J. Early Adolesc. 35, 905–930. https​://doi.org/10.1177/02724​ 31614​54704​9 (2015). 36. Neumann, A., van Lier, P. A. C., Frijns, T., Meeus, W. & Koot, H. M. Emotional dynamics in the development of early adolescent psychopathology: A one-year longitudinal Study. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 39, 657–669. https:​ //doi.org/10.1007/s10802​ -011-9509-3 (2011). 37. Hox, J., Moerbeek, M. & van de Schoot, R. Multilevel Analysis: Techniques and Applications 3rd edn. (Routledge, London, 2018). 38. Wang, L. P. & Maxwell, S. E. On disaggregating between-person and within-person effects with longitudinal data using multilevel models. Psychol. Methods 20, 63–83. https​://doi.org/10.1037/met00​00030​ (2015). 39. Satorra, A. & Bentler, P. M. Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic. Psychometrika 75, 243–248. https​ ://doi.org/10.1007/s1133​6-009-9135-y (2010). 40. Schwarz, G. Estimating the dimension of a model. Ann. Stat. 6, 461–464. https​://doi.org/10.1214/aos/11763​44136​ (1978). 41. Akaike, H. A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control 19, 716–723. https​://doi.org/10.1109/ TAC.1974.11007​05 (1974). 42. Keijsers, L. et al. What drives developmental change in adolescent disclosure and maternal knowledge? Heterogeneity in withinfamily processes. Dev. Psychol. 52, 2057–2070. https​://doi.org/10.1037/dev00​00220​ (2016). 43. R Core Team R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, 2017). 44. Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. Mplus User’s Guide 8th edn. (Muthén & Muthén, Los Angeles, 2017). 45. Beyens, I., Pouwels, J. L., van Driel, I. I., Keijsers, L. & Valkenburg, P. M. Dataset belonging to Beyens et al. (2020). The effect of social media on well-being differs from adolescent to adolescent. https​://doi.org/10.21942​/uva.12497​990 (2020). Scientific Reports | Vol:.(1234567890) (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 10 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Acknowledgements This study was funded by the NWO Spinoza Prize and the Gravitation grant (NWO Grant 024.001.003; Consortium on Individual Development) awarded to P.M.V. by the Dutch Research Council (NWO). Additional funding was received from the VIDI grant (NWO VIDI Grant 452.17.011) awarded to L.K. by the Dutch Research Council (NWO). The authors would like to thank Savannah Boele (Tilburg University) for providing her pilot ESM results. Author contributions I.B., J.L.P., I.I.v.D., L.K., and P.M.V. designed the study; I.B., J.L.P., and I.I.v.D. collected the data; I.B., J.L.P., and L.K. analyzed the data; and I.B., J.L.P., I.I.v.D., L.K., and P.M.V. contributed to writing and reviewing the manuscript. Competing interests The authors declare no competing interests. Additional information Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to I.B. Reprints and permissions information is available at www.nature.com/reprints. Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creat​iveco​mmons​.org/licen​ses/by/4.0/. © The Author(s) 2020 Scientific Reports | (2020) 10:10763 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-67727-7 11 Vol.:(0123456789)

Are you stuck with your online class?
Get help from our team of writers!

Order your essay today and save 20% with the discount code RAPID